tomwalters@0
|
1 .TH GENSPL 1 "8 September 1993"
|
tomwalters@0
|
2 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
3 .SH NAME
|
tomwalters@0
|
4 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
5 genspl \- spiral auditory image of a pulse train
|
tomwalters@0
|
6 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
7 .SH SYNOPSIS/SYNTAX
|
tomwalters@0
|
8 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
9 genspl [ option=value | -option ] filename
|
tomwalters@0
|
10 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
11 .SH DESCRIPTION
|
tomwalters@0
|
12 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
13 Since the spiral auditory image is just a different view of the
|
tomwalters@0
|
14 auditory image, it includes all of the flags associated
|
tomwalters@0
|
15 previously with the gensai command. In the ASP software, the
|
tomwalters@0
|
16 spiral auditory image is presented in cartoon form, similar to
|
tomwalters@0
|
17 the presentation of the linear auditory image. The spiral view
|
tomwalters@0
|
18 of the auditory image is a global view of the sound that
|
tomwalters@0
|
19 emphasises pitch and de-emphasises timbre. It is a distant
|
tomwalters@0
|
20 perspective taken in order to view the longer term correlations
|
tomwalters@0
|
21 that arise in periodic sounds. It is difficult to represent the
|
tomwalters@0
|
22 functions of the SAI visually in a spiral form; the fine detail
|
tomwalters@0
|
23 of the functions wouldbe lost in the spiral perspective.
|
tomwalters@0
|
24 Accordingly, in the spiral perspective each of the separate SAI
|
tomwalters@0
|
25 pulses is replaced by a dot positioned at the time of the peak
|
tomwalters@0
|
26 of the pulse. Previously, this representation was referred to
|
tomwalters@0
|
27 as a pulse ribbon (Patterson, 1987a).
|
tomwalters@0
|
28 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
29 Conceptually, the spiral auditory image is a set of concentric
|
tomwalters@0
|
30 spirals one for each channel of the auditory image. The highest
|
tomwalters@0
|
31 frequency channel is on the inside with the smallest radius; the
|
tomwalters@0
|
32 lowest frequency channel is on the outside with the largest
|
tomwalters@0
|
33 radius. The spirals lines are omitted for clarity, leaving just
|
tomwalters@0
|
34 the dots. The presence of bars shows that the same period exists
|
tomwalters@0
|
35 in a range of filter channels. Note, however, that this
|
tomwalters@0
|
36 information about correlation across channels appears on the same
|
tomwalters@0
|
37 spoke as the information indicating that the pattern repeats on
|
tomwalters@0
|
38 the auditory image in time. Thus the multi-channel spiral maps
|
tomwalters@0
|
39 both spectral and temporal information concerning the
|
tomwalters@0
|
40 periodicity of the sound onto a single spatial vector -- a spoke
|
tomwalters@0
|
41 of the spiral. It is this property that enables the spiral
|
tomwalters@0
|
42 representation to explain octave perception (Patterson, 1990).
|
tomwalters@0
|
43 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
44 .SS "A pitch glide in the spiral auditory image "
|
tomwalters@0
|
45 .PP
|
tomwalters@0
|
46 The spiral auditory image, like its linear counterpart, is not
|
tomwalters@0
|
47 limited to periodic sounds. When the pitch of a sound glides
|
tomwalters@0
|
48 smoothly from one note to another the pattern on the spiral
|
tomwalters@0
|
49 auditory image rotates smoothly from one position to another, and
|
tomwalters@0
|
50 when the pitch changes abruptly from one note to another, the
|
tomwalters@0
|
51 spiral pattern dissolves at the end of the first note and forms
|
tomwalters@0
|
52 again in a different orientation at the start of the second note.
|
tomwalters@0
|
53 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
54 The spiral spokes grow from the centre outwards as the
|
tomwalters@0
|
55 correlation across cycles grows. For the note C3, four spokes
|
tomwalters@0
|
56 form: the vertical spoke contains information about periods
|
tomwalters@0
|
57 separated by 1, 2, 4 and 8 cycles; the spoke at 25 minutes past
|
tomwalters@0
|
58 the hour contains information about periods separated by 3 and 6
|
tomwalters@0
|
59 cycles; the remaining two spokes at 40 and 10 minutes past the
|
tomwalters@0
|
60 hour contain information about periods separated by 5 and 7
|
tomwalters@0
|
61 cycles, respectively.
|
tomwalters@0
|
62 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
63 As the pitch of the note changes from C3 to E3, the pattern
|
tomwalters@0
|
64 rotates 20 minutes, and the spoke that was previously at 40
|
tomwalters@0
|
65 minutes moves into the vertical position. Then, as the pitch
|
tomwalters@0
|
66 glides from E3 to G3, the spoke which was at 25 minutes in C3,
|
tomwalters@0
|
67 moves into the vertical position. As the pitch glides on up from
|
tomwalters@0
|
68 G3 to C4 the longest spoke of the pattern returns to the vertical
|
tomwalters@0
|
69 position completing one revolution as the pitch rises an octave.
|
tomwalters@0
|
70 Note, however, that each of the spokes has been extended by one
|
tomwalters@0
|
71 circuit towards the centre of the spiral. Thus, in the ASP model,
|
tomwalters@0
|
72 octaves are perceived to be similar because they produce spoke
|
tomwalters@0
|
73 patterns with the same orientation on the spiral auditory image
|
tomwalters@0
|
74 and the notes of the major triad are those with a spoke that
|
tomwalters@0
|
75 coincides with the main spoke of the tonic. A theory of musical
|
tomwalters@0
|
76 consonance based on the coincidence of spokes in spiral auditory
|
tomwalters@0
|
77 images is presented in Patterson (1986).
|
tomwalters@0
|
78 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
79 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
80 .SH OPTIONS
|
tomwalters@0
|
81 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
82 .SS "Display options for the spiral auditory image "
|
tomwalters@0
|
83 .PP
|
tomwalters@0
|
84 The options that control the position of the spiral image window
|
tomwalters@0
|
85 on the screen are the same as for all previous windows.
|
tomwalters@0
|
86 Furthermore, since the spiral auditory image is a cartoon just
|
tomwalters@0
|
87 like the linear auditory image, it may be generated, stored,
|
tomwalters@0
|
88 animated, and reviewed in the same way as the linear auditory
|
tomwalters@0
|
89 image. In addition, there are six new display options for the
|
tomwalters@0
|
90 spiral view of the auditory image.
|
tomwalters@0
|
91 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
92 .TP 11
|
tomwalters@0
|
93 spiral
|
tomwalters@0
|
94 Switch to spiral auditory image
|
tomwalters@0
|
95 .RS
|
tomwalters@0
|
96 Switch: Default, off.
|
tomwalters@0
|
97 .RE
|
tomwalters@0
|
98 .RS
|
tomwalters@0
|
99 When spiral is set to "on" the time dimension of the auditory image is plotted as a spiral and the SAI function is replaced with dots positioned at the peaks of the pulses in the SAI function.
|
tomwalters@0
|
100 .RE
|
tomwalters@0
|
101 .TP 13
|
tomwalters@0
|
102 form_spl
|
tomwalters@0
|
103 The form of the spiral time line
|
tomwalters@0
|
104 .RS
|
tomwalters@0
|
105 Switch: Default, archimedian.
|
tomwalters@0
|
106 .RE
|
tomwalters@0
|
107 .RS
|
tomwalters@0
|
108 The software offers two visual representations of the underlying logarithmic spiral, both of which have the base 2.
|
tomwalters@0
|
109 .RE
|
tomwalters@0
|
110 Both representations gather doublings in time onto a specific
|
tomwalters@0
|
111 spoke of the spiral, and so both have the general property that
|
tomwalters@0
|
112 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
113 q = log2(t/T) (6.1)
|
tomwalters@0
|
114 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
115 q is the angle between the horizontal axis and the radius drawn
|
tomwalters@0
|
116 to point on the spiral. T is the period of the sampling rate and
|
tomwalters@0
|
117 t is "auditory image time", both in seconds. Every time t doubles
|
tomwalters@0
|
118 q increases by 1, and so the integer part of q (the characteristic
|
tomwalters@0
|
119 of the logarithm) specifies the circuit of the spiral. The
|
tomwalters@0
|
120 fractional part of the logarithm (the mantissa) specifies the
|
tomwalters@0
|
121 angle within the circuit, and in this case, the angle is measured
|
tomwalters@0
|
122 in revolutions, or circuits.
|
tomwalters@0
|
123 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
124 The archimedian spiral is like a coil of rope; that is, the radius
|
tomwalters@0
|
125 increases by the thickness of the rope on each successive
|
tomwalters@0
|
126 circuit. The form of the archimedian spiral is
|
tomwalters@0
|
127 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
128 r = aq = a log2(t/T) (6.2)
|
tomwalters@0
|
129 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
130 where r is the radius from the centre of the spiral to a point
|
tomwalters@0
|
131 on the spiral. The logarithmic spiral has the form
|
tomwalters@0
|
132 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
133 r = 2q = 2log2(t/T) = t/T (6.3)
|
tomwalters@0
|
134 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
135 The logarithmic version of the spiral has the advantage that
|
tomwalters@0
|
136 image time is linear along the path of the spiral. However, it
|
tomwalters@0
|
137 has the disadvantage that it expands rapidly, and so the current
|
tomwalters@0
|
138 default is archimedian.
|
tomwalters@0
|
139 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
140 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
141 .TP 16
|
tomwalters@0
|
142 dotsize_spl
|
tomwalters@0
|
143 The size of the dots on the spiral
|
tomwalters@0
|
144 .RS
|
tomwalters@0
|
145 Default units, pixels: Default value, 2 pixels.
|
tomwalters@0
|
146 .RE
|
tomwalters@0
|
147 .RS
|
tomwalters@0
|
148 The dots plotted on the spiral are actually small squares and the value dotsize_spl determines the number of pixels along the side of the square.
|
tomwalters@0
|
149 .RE
|
tomwalters@0
|
150 .TP 13
|
tomwalters@0
|
151 axis_spl
|
tomwalters@0
|
152 Spiral axis, or time line
|
tomwalters@0
|
153 .RS
|
tomwalters@0
|
154 Switch: Default, off
|
tomwalters@0
|
155 .RE
|
tomwalters@0
|
156 .RS
|
tomwalters@0
|
157 When the axis_spl switch is set to "on", a spiral axis, or time line is plotted. It is presented on the outside of the circuit, one channel below the lowest filter channel, just as in the linear image. The default value for axis_spl is "off" because the spiral axis contains a large number of points and it is slow to calculate and plot.
|
tomwalters@0
|
158 .RE
|
tomwalters@0
|
159 Note: The length of spiral displayed in the window is determined
|
tomwalters@0
|
160 by duration_sai. This is the same duration_sai as for the linear
|
tomwalters@0
|
161 image. The size of the spiral display is scaled so that the
|
tomwalters@0
|
162 radius associated with duration_sai fits inside the rectangle
|
tomwalters@0
|
163 specified for the window. The spiral does not have to be
|
tomwalters@0
|
164 presented in a square window and in some instances rectangular
|
tomwalters@0
|
165 windows are quite effective for giving a sense of depth.
|
tomwalters@0
|
166 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
167 .TP 13
|
tomwalters@0
|
168 zero_spl
|
tomwalters@0
|
169 Spiral start point and spiral orientation
|
tomwalters@0
|
170 .RS
|
tomwalters@0
|
171 Default units: revolutions. Default value 4.072 revolutions.
|
tomwalters@0
|
172 .RE
|
tomwalters@0
|
173 .RS
|
tomwalters@0
|
174 This parameter determines the minimum "auditory image time" that appears on the spiral, and thus it determines the zero point on the spiral.
|
tomwalters@0
|
175 .RE
|
tomwalters@0
|
176 The parameter zero_spl has two primary uses: Firstly, it enables
|
tomwalters@0
|
177 the user to determine the orientation of the main spoke of the
|
tomwalters@0
|
178 spiral for a given combination of sampling rate and stimulus
|
tomwalters@0
|
179 period. Without the parameter zero_spl, the orientation of the
|
tomwalters@0
|
180 spiral would be fixed by the sampling rate and period of the
|
tomwalters@0
|
181 sound. Periods that are an exact power-of-2 times the base
|
tomwalters@0
|
182 period, 1/T, would appear on the spoke preceding horizontally
|
tomwalters@0
|
183 from the centre of the spiral towards the right. By removing a
|
tomwalters@0
|
184 portion of a circuit the orientation of the spiral can be set to
|
tomwalters@0
|
185 suit the user. A reduction in zero_spl of 0.25 will rotate the
|
tomwalters@0
|
186 main spoke from horizontal to vertical.
|
tomwalters@0
|
187 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
188 The second purpose of zero_spl is to enable the user to adjust
|
tomwalters@0
|
189 the image to the period being displayed; that is, to focus on the
|
tomwalters@0
|
190 octave of the current sound. For example, when the sound has a
|
tomwalters@0
|
191 long period, like 8 ms, the activity produced by the sound falls
|
tomwalters@0
|
192 in the outer circuits of the spiral. If zero_spl is set to a
|
tomwalters@0
|
193 small value (<2) the centre of the display will be largely blank.
|
tomwalters@0
|
194 The short circuits associated with higher octaves can be removed
|
tomwalters@0
|
195 by setting zero_spl to a larger value, say 4, in which case a
|
tomwalters@0
|
196 sound with an 8 ms period will fill the display.
|
tomwalters@0
|
197 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
198 The one parameter zero_spl can be used to both scale and rotate
|
tomwalters@0
|
199 the spiral simultaneously; integer changes in the parameter cause
|
tomwalters@0
|
200 a scaling without rotation. The default value, 4.072, assigns a
|
tomwalters@0
|
201 vertical spoke to a period of 8 ms (and its base-2 relatives)
|
tomwalters@0
|
202 when the sampling rate is 20 kHz (or a base-2 relative).
|
tomwalters@0
|
203 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
204 .TP 18
|
tomwalters@0
|
205 dotthresh_spl
|
tomwalters@0
|
206 Threshold value for the production of a spiral dot
|
tomwalters@0
|
207 .RS
|
tomwalters@0
|
208 Unit: SAI strength. Default value, 50 SAI units.
|
tomwalters@0
|
209 .RE
|
tomwalters@0
|
210 .RS
|
tomwalters@0
|
211 This threshold specifies the value that a pulse in the SAI must reach, or exceeds in order for it to be presented as a dot in the spiral image.
|
tomwalters@0
|
212 .RE
|
tomwalters@0
|
213 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
214 .SH EXAMPLES
|
tomwalters@0
|
215 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
216 In order to understand the spiral mapping, look at the auditory
|
tomwalters@0
|
217 image of C3 and imagine the pulse ribbon that would be formed by
|
tomwalters@0
|
218 replacing each SAI pulse with a dot and extending the duration
|
tomwalters@0
|
219 of the image to 70 ms so that it will accommodate eight cycles
|
tomwalters@0
|
220 of the note. The spiral view is produced by compressing the pulse
|
tomwalters@0
|
221 ribbon vertically, stretching it horizontally, and then wrapping
|
tomwalters@0
|
222 it counterclockwise into a spiral, with the right-hand edge at
|
tomwalters@0
|
223 the centre of the spiral and the left-hand edge at the end of the
|
tomwalters@0
|
224 outer circuit. The dots from vertical columns of pulses in the
|
tomwalters@0
|
225 linear auditory image, merge into short bars in the spiral view
|
tomwalters@0
|
226 because of the vertical compression; the bars fall along spokes
|
tomwalters@0
|
227 radiating from the centre of the spiral. The dots from the arches
|
tomwalters@0
|
228 of pulses on either side of the vertical column in the linear
|
tomwalters@0
|
229 auditory image appear in a stretched form like "wings" in the
|
tomwalters@0
|
230 spiral auditory image.
|
tomwalters@0
|
231 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
232 In the case of C3 four of the bars are aligned on one spoke of
|
tomwalters@0
|
233 the spiral (the vertical spoke); they represent the strong
|
tomwalters@0
|
234 correlations that occur in the auditory image for cycles of the
|
tomwalters@0
|
235 original sound separated by 1, 2, 4, and 8 cycles. In this way,
|
tomwalters@0
|
236 much of the information that is distributed across the temporal
|
tomwalters@0
|
237 dimension of the linear auditory image is gathered together into
|
tomwalters@0
|
238 a single spatial vector.
|
tomwalters@0
|
239 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
240 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
241 The wave cegc provides an example of how the spiral auditory
|
tomwalters@0
|
242 image follows pitch glides from one note to another. One
|
tomwalters@0
|
243 reasonable version of the spiral pitch glide is provided by the
|
tomwalters@0
|
244 command
|
tomwalters@0
|
245 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
246 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
247 genspl width=600 height=550 duration_sai=70 zero_spl=5.072 cegc
|
tomwalters@0
|
248 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
249 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
250 .SS "The separation of pitch and timbre in the auditory image. "
|
tomwalters@0
|
251 .PP
|
tomwalters@0
|
252 The file vowgld contains a synthetic speech waveform that
|
tomwalters@0
|
253 combines both formant motion and pitch motion; the formant motion
|
tomwalters@0
|
254 is a rapid tour around the vowel triangle as in aiua, and the
|
tomwalters@0
|
255 pitch motion is C3, E3, G3 and C4. A linear auditory image of
|
tomwalters@0
|
256 vowgld can be generated with the command
|
tomwalters@0
|
257 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
258 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
259 gensai width=420 height=420 mag=12 segment=40 duration_sai=20
|
tomwalters@0
|
260 spiral=off vowgld
|
tomwalters@0
|
261 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
262 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
263 The motion in the linear auditory image is similar to that
|
tomwalters@0
|
264 observed with aiua in Chapter 5. That is, the formants move
|
tomwalters@0
|
265 vertically as the vowels change from one to the next. In this
|
tomwalters@0
|
266 example, however, there is pitch motion and the period decreases
|
tomwalters@0
|
267 by a factor of 2 as the example proceeds. The pitch change is
|
tomwalters@0
|
268 observed primarily as horizontal motion that is largely
|
tomwalters@0
|
269 independent of the formant motion. In point of fact, the
|
tomwalters@0
|
270 resolved harmonics in the lower half of the auditory image are
|
tomwalters@0
|
271 rising in frequency as the example proceeds but this does not
|
tomwalters@0
|
272 seem to interfere with the perception of either the vertical
|
tomwalters@0
|
273 motion of the formants or the horizontal shrinking of the
|
tomwalters@0
|
274 period.
|
tomwalters@0
|
275 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
276 Although the rise in pitch can be observed in the linear auditory
|
tomwalters@0
|
277 image it is not the dominant perception; rather, it is the
|
tomwalters@0
|
278 formant motion that dominates in this microscopic view of the
|
tomwalters@0
|
279 auditory image. A spiral auditory image of vowgld can be
|
tomwalters@0
|
280 generated with the command
|
tomwalters@0
|
281 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
282 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
283 gensai width=420 height=420 segment=40 duration_sai=70 spiral=on
|
tomwalters@0
|
284 zero_spl=5.072 vowgld
|
tomwalters@0
|
285 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
286 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
287 The motion in the spiral auditory image is dominated by the
|
tomwalters@0
|
288 rotation of the spokes, that is, the pitch motion. The motion
|
tomwalters@0
|
289 of the formants is represented in the spiral image in the sense
|
tomwalters@0
|
290 that there is more sparkle in the information that is not on the
|
tomwalters@0
|
291 main spoke pattern. This sparkle is caused by the formant energy
|
tomwalters@0
|
292 changing channels as the formants move from channel to channel
|
tomwalters@0
|
293 within one circuit of the spiral. But the fact that the motion
|
tomwalters@0
|
294 in successive circuits is coordinated is not apparent in this
|
tomwalters@0
|
295 macro view of the auditory image.
|
tomwalters@0
|
296 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
297 A more dramatic example of the enhancement pitch and the
|
tomwalters@0
|
298 repression of timbre can be produced by generating a spiral
|
tomwalters@0
|
299 auditory image for aiua in which the pitch is fixed and the
|
tomwalters@0
|
300 vowels range around the vowel triangle. The formant information
|
tomwalters@0
|
301 on the spokes changes as the vowel tour proceeds but the position
|
tomwalters@0
|
302 of the spokes remains fixed. The vowel information of the spokes
|
tomwalters@0
|
303 rushes around in three discrete transitions but there is no
|
tomwalters@0
|
304 particular pattern to the motion.
|
tomwalters@0
|
305 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
306 Thus, in the ASP model, pitch and timbre are just two views of
|
tomwalters@0
|
307 the same auditory image; pitch effects are observed when we stand
|
tomwalters@0
|
308 back and take a macroscopic view of the auditory image; timbre
|
tomwalters@0
|
309 details are observed when we move in close and take a microscopic
|
tomwalters@0
|
310 view of the auditory image.
|
tomwalters@0
|
311 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
312 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
313 The review program has the capacity to present two auditory
|
tomwalters@0
|
314 images simultaneously. If linear and spiral auditory images of
|
tomwalters@0
|
315 vowgld are generated and stored using image=on, they can be
|
tomwalters@0
|
316 replayed simultaneously and compared using the command
|
tomwalters@0
|
317 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
318 review vowgld_l vowgld_s
|
tomwalters@0
|
319 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
320 Caution: this requires the user to produce separate image files
|
tomwalters@0
|
321 (vowgld_l.img, vowgld_s.img) either by producing the images from
|
tomwalters@0
|
322 copies of vowgld with different names, or by renaming the
|
tomwalters@0
|
323 auditory images as they are produced. If two different auditory
|
tomwalters@0
|
324 images are produced from the same file, the second will overwrite
|
tomwalters@0
|
325 the first even though one has a linear format and one a spiral
|
tomwalters@0
|
326 format.
|
tomwalters@0
|
327 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
328 .SS "Multiple pitches in the spiral auditory image "
|
tomwalters@0
|
329 .PP
|
tomwalters@0
|
330 It is generally assumed that when two people are speaking at the
|
tomwalters@0
|
331 same time, the listener uses the differences in the pitches of
|
tomwalters@0
|
332 the two voices to assist in separating the speakers. The final
|
tomwalters@0
|
333 example in this chapter shows that the pitches of the /a/ and the
|
tomwalters@0
|
334 /o/ in dblvow appear separately in the spiral auditory image, and
|
tomwalters@0
|
335 that it would be reasonable to use the spiral to separate the
|
tomwalters@0
|
336 channels associated with the two vowels and thereby assist
|
tomwalters@0
|
337 speaker tracking. The spiral auditory image can be generated by
|
tomwalters@0
|
338 the command
|
tomwalters@0
|
339 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
340 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
341 gensai width=600 height=550 samplerate=10000 spiral=on
|
tomwalters@0
|
342 duration=90 dblvow
|
tomwalters@0
|
343 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
344 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
345 The main spokes of the /a/ and the /i/ appear at angles of 40 and
|
tomwalters@0
|
346 0 minutes past the hour, respectively, corresponding to periods
|
tomwalters@0
|
347 of 10 and 8 ms. Over the course of the example, the main spoke
|
tomwalters@0
|
348 of the /i/ fades considerably while the main spoke of the /a/
|
tomwalters@0
|
349 increases somewhat.
|
tomwalters@0
|
350 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
351 The second spoke of the /a/ and /i/ patterns appear at 5 and 25
|
tomwalters@0
|
352 minutes, respectively, and their strength changes predictably
|
tomwalters@0
|
353 as the example proceeds. If either vowel were presented on its
|
tomwalters@0
|
354 own there would be more than two spokes in the pattern of each
|
tomwalters@0
|
355 vowel. The presence of the second vowel represses spokes beyond
|
tomwalters@0
|
356 the second in the patterns of both vowels.
|
tomwalters@0
|
357 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
358 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
359 .SH BUGS
|
tomwalters@0
|
360 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
361 Note: the current vrsion of the software (release 3, June 1990)
|
tomwalters@0
|
362 incorrectly adds linear axes to hardcopy figures. Apologies.
|
tomwalters@0
|
363 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
364 .SH COPYRIGHT
|
tomwalters@0
|
365 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
366 Copyright (c) Applied Psychology Unit, Medical Research Council, 1995
|
tomwalters@0
|
367 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
368 Permission to use, copy, modify, and distribute this software without fee
|
tomwalters@0
|
369 is hereby granted for research purposes, provided that this copyright
|
tomwalters@0
|
370 notice appears in all copies and in all supporting documentation, and that
|
tomwalters@0
|
371 the software is not redistributed for any fee (except for a nominal
|
tomwalters@0
|
372 shipping charge). Anyone wanting to incorporate all or part of this
|
tomwalters@0
|
373 software in a commercial product must obtain a license from the Medical
|
tomwalters@0
|
374 Research Council.
|
tomwalters@0
|
375 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
376 The MRC makes no representations about the suitability of this
|
tomwalters@0
|
377 software for any purpose. It is provided "as is" without express or
|
tomwalters@0
|
378 implied warranty.
|
tomwalters@0
|
379 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
380 THE MRC DISCLAIMS ALL WARRANTIES WITH REGARD TO THIS SOFTWARE, INCLUDING
|
tomwalters@0
|
381 ALL IMPLIED WARRANTIES OF MERCHANTABILITY AND FITNESS, IN NO EVENT SHALL
|
tomwalters@0
|
382 THE A.P.U. BE LIABLE FOR ANY SPECIAL, INDIRECT OR CONSEQUENTIAL DAMAGES
|
tomwalters@0
|
383 OR ANY DAMAGES WHATSOEVER RESULTING FROM LOSS OF USE, DATA OR PROFITS,
|
tomwalters@0
|
384 WHETHER IN AN ACTION OF CONTRACT, NEGLIGENCE OR OTHER TORTIOUS ACTION,
|
tomwalters@0
|
385 ARISING OUT OF OR IN CONNECTION WITH THE USE OR PERFORMANCE OF THIS
|
tomwalters@0
|
386 SOFTWARE.
|
tomwalters@0
|
387 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
388 .SH ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
|
tomwalters@0
|
389 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
390 The AIM software was developed for Unix workstations by John
|
tomwalters@0
|
391 Holdsworth and Mike Allerhand of the MRC APU, under the direction of
|
tomwalters@0
|
392 Roy Patterson. The physiological version of AIM was developed by
|
tomwalters@0
|
393 Christian Giguere. The options handler is by Paul Manson. The revised
|
tomwalters@0
|
394 SAI module is by Jay Datta. Michael Akeroyd extended the postscript
|
tomwalters@0
|
395 facilites and developed the xreview routine for auditory image
|
tomwalters@0
|
396 cartoons.
|
tomwalters@0
|
397 .LP
|
tomwalters@0
|
398 The project was supported by the MRC and grants from the U.K. Defense
|
tomwalters@0
|
399 Research Agency, Farnborough (Research Contract 2239); the EEC Esprit
|
tomwalters@0
|
400 BR Porgramme, Project ACTS (3207); and the U.K. Hearing Research Trust.
|
tomwalters@0
|
401
|